JEPonline
Journal
of
Exercise
Physiologyonline
ISSN
1097-9751
An
International Electronic
Journal
for Exercise Physiologists
Vol 1 No 1 April 1998
|
Metabolic Responses
To Exercise
The
effect of psychophysiologic self-regulation on
running economy
Tommy Boone and Jeanne DeWeese
Professor and Chair
Department of Exercise Physiology
The College of St. Scholastica
Duluth, MN 55811
BOONE, T. and J. DeWEESE. The effect
of psychophysiologic self-regulation on running economy. JEPonline,
Vol. 1, No. 1, 1998. The purpose of this study
was to determine the physiological effects of eliciting the relaxation
response during exercise. Nine adult females volunteered to participate
in this study. The subjects received 30 minutes of progressive muscle relaxation
(PMR) instructions per session for eight sessions. During the week
following PMR, the subjects exercised for 30 minutes of continuous activity
on the treadmill. The first and third 10 minutes of exercise were
control periods. During the second 10 minutes (treatment period),
the subjects elicited the relaxation response. Oxygen consumption
and related measures were determined using the Beckman Metabolic Measurement
Cart. A repeated measures ANOVA was used to analyze the data.
During the treatment period, there were significant (p<0.05) decreases
in Fb, Ve,
SBP, and RPP when compared to the two control periods. There were no significant
(p>0.05) differences in Vt,
VO2, VCO2,
RER, and HR. This study showed that the elicitation of the relaxation response
during exercise did not decrease submaximal VO2 and, therefore,
did not alter running economy. Statistically significant changes
in ventilation and blood pressure were associated with the elicitation
of the relaxation response during exercise. Regarding the latter
findings, there is ample evidence that a reduction in RPP has a positive
and unequivocal beneficial influence on the work of the heart during exercise.
Key Words: SELF-REGULATION,
OXYGEN CONSUMPTION, VENTILATION, BLOOD
PRESSURE, RUNNING ECONOMY
Introduction
The purpose of psychophysiologic self-regulation
is to learn conscious control of the autonomic nervous system in order
to bring involuntary body responses (i.e., respiration, oxygen uptake,
heart rate, and blood pressure) under voluntary control (1,2).
Self-regulation of the visceral and somatic responses can be achieved with
the practice of one or more biobehavioral interventions such as relaxation,
autogenics, biofeedback, meditation, imagery, prayer, and music.
The result may be the learned control to reverse the negative effects of
cardiovascular disease, evoke positive body-mind-spirit responses to stressors,
or to enhance well-being through increased inner peace and calmness.
Regardless of the biobehavioral intervention
used, the psychophysiologic effects on the cardiovascular system are consistent
with decreased sympathetic nervous system activity (3,6)
and decreased metabolism (3,4,7-9).
These responses highlight the influence of psychological states on physiological
processes at rest (10,11).
Few studies have examined the effects of biobehavioral interventions during
exercise. Benson, Dryer, and Hartley
(12) reported decreased
metabolism during exercise with elicitation of the relaxation response.
The subjects had a minimum of 6 months of relaxation training. Oxygen consumption
(VO2) decreased an average of 4% during
the exercise period with relaxation. Gervino and Veazey
(13)
extended the work of Benson and colleagues (12) and
found that the elicitation of the relaxation response during exercise resulted
in a significant decrease in the physiological cost (VO2)
of a given workload of submaximalrunning. Ziegler, Klinzing, and
Williamson
(14) also reported a decrease in metabolism
(VO2) during a submaximal run that incorporated
cognitive coping strategies and relaxation. On the other hand, Smith,
Gill, Crews, Hopewell, and Morgan (15) reported no significant
physiological changes in VO2 and heart
rate (HR) when using relaxation during distance running. In agreement,
Ashley, Rajab, Timmons, Smith, and Mutrie (16) found
that 2 weeks of self-instruction in progressive muscular relaxation (PMR)
had no significant effects on VO2, HR,
and expired ventilation (Ve) during submaximal
running. These findings are also consistent with earlier reports
by Cadarette et al. (17) and Cortes, Boyd, and Boone
(18).
This lack of consensus reflects the limited
availability of research examining this topic and the ambiguity that exists
in the literature. If the volume of oxygen consumed at a fixed work
intensity can be decreased with elicitation of the relaxation response
and if the physiological variables, HR and VO2,
can be used to assess cardiorespiratory efficiency (i.e., exercise economy),
then psychophysiologic self-regulation may translate into improved running
performance. Here, the improvement in economy is defined as using
a lower percentage of maximal oxygen consumption (VO2max)
at a given submaximal running velocity (19,20).
The purpose of this study was to determine
the physiological effects of eliciting the relaxation response during exercise.
Our null hypothesis was that in healthy female subjects the learned self-
regulation of the autonomic nervous system at rest would not be under voluntary
control during exercise.
Methods
Subjects
Nine sedentary adult females (Mean age
= 23±2 years; Mean mass = 59±2.9 kg; Men height = 164.2±0.8
cm) volunteered to participate in this study. None of the subjects
was engaged in a regular exercise program or relaxation training prior
to the experiment. The subjects gave their informed consent, and were informed
of the test procedures and purpose of the study.
Experimental Procedure
Relaxation Procedures. During the
orientation day, the subjects were given verbal instructions and familiarization
with a progressive muscle relaxation (PMR) strategy designed to elicit
parasympathetic dominance. This strategy required the subjects (under
the direction of the instructor) to consciously tense and relax major muscle
groups throughout the body to become more aware of subtle degrees of tension.
The subjects received 30 minutes of PMR instructions per session for eight
sessions (using a Monday, Wednesday, and Friday schedule). Heart
rate, systolic blood pressure (SBP), and rate pressure product (RPP) were
recorded at scheduled intervals to validate the PMR strategy. The
PMR sessions resulted in significant (p<.05) decreases in HR (74±9
to 63±8 beats/min), SBP (122±10 to 110±12 mm Hg),
and RPP (90±6 to 69±8), respectively from the first PMR session
to the eighth PMR session.
Familiarization. On Friday of the
third week of the study (following the eighth PMR session), each subject
reported to the laboratory to receive a general explanation of the testing
protocol and to participate in a 10-minute simulation of the actual treadmill
exercise. At 0% grade, treadmill speed was determined for each subject
to approximate 60% age-predicted maximum HR (using 220-age), and to standardize
the relative intensity for all subjects.
Test Protocol. During
the week following the PMR instructions, the subjects returned to the laboratory
for 30 minutes of continuous treadmill exercise. During minutes 1-10,
the subjects exercised without eliciting the relaxation response (Control
I). During minutes 11-20, the subjects elicited the relaxation response
(Treatment). During minutes 21-30, the subjects continued to exercise
but without eliciting the relaxation response (Control II).
Physiological Measures. Frequency
of breaths (Fb), tidal volume (Vt),
expired ventilation (Ve), VO2,
carbon dioxide production (VCO2), and respiratory
exchange ratio (RER) were determined by the Beckman Metabolic Measurement
Cart (MMC), which was calibrated prior to and checked after each test session
with standardized reference gases. Heart rate was determined by 10-second
electrocardiographic strips using a modified CM5 lead. Only the values
during the second 5 minutes of each 10-minute period were averaged and
statistically compared. Systolic blood pressure was determined indirectly
during minutes 10, 20, and 30 by auscultation of the left brachial artery
using a standard sphygmomanometer. Rate pressure product (also commonly
referred to as double product) was calculated from the formula: (RPP =
HR x SBP x .01).
Statistical Analysis
To verify that the subjects were able
to elicit the relaxation response following the eight PMR sessions, physiological
data from the first and eighth sessions were statistically compared using
a two-tailed paired t-test. An analysis of variance with repeated
measures was used to assess the mean difference for each variable across
the three 10-minute exercise periods. Where indicated, a Newman Kuels
post hoc analysis was used to determine the significant differences among
the means. An alpha of 0.05 probability level was used for all tests
of statistical significance.
Results
Means and standard deviations were computed
for all physiological data (Table 1). Statistical analysis indicated significant
differences (p<0.05) in the Treatment values for Fb,
Ve, SBP, and RPP versus Control I and/or
II. There were no significant differences (p>0.05) in Vt,
VO2, VCO2,
RER, and HR.
Table 1. Cardiorespiratory and hemodynamic
responses at a fixed work intensity before (Control I), during (Treatment),
and after (Control II) the subjects were told to try and elicit the relaxation
response (M±SD).
Variable |
Control I
(A) |
Treatment
(B) |
Control II
(C) |
F-ratio
& Prob |
Fb
breaths/min |
31 ± 8
A-B** |
28 ± 7
|
30 ± 8
|
5.68 & .01*
|
Vt
ml/breath |
784 ± 95
|
814 ± 115
|
797 ± 104
|
1.63 & .22
|
Ve
l/min |
24 ± 3
A-B**
A-C** |
23 ± 3
|
23 ± 3
|
6.06 & .01*
|
VO2
l/min |
.79 ± .06
|
.78 ± .05
|
.77 ± .05
|
2.15 & .14
|
VCO2
l/min |
.67 ± .07
|
.66 ± .07
|
.65 ± .06
|
1.91 & .18
|
RER |
.85 ± .04 |
.85 ± .04 |
.84 ± .05 |
.25 & .78 |
HR
beats/min |
125 ± 13
|
122 ± 9
|
122 ±15
|
3.00 & .07
|
SBP
mmHg |
132 ± 13
A-B** |
124 ± 9
B-C** |
132 ± 12
|
4.08 & .03*
|
DP |
165 ± 22
A-B** |
151 ± 18
B-C** |
161 ±18
|
3.86 & .04*
|
*ANOVA with repeated
measures
**Newman Kuels post hoc
analysis (p<0.05)
Discussion
The major finding of this study was that
the elicitation of the relaxation response during submaximal treadmill
exercise did not result in a significant decrease in VO2.
The null hypothesis was supported. The subjects were not able to
improve their running economy (i.e., decrease VO2)
during the exercise period in which psychophysiologic self-regulation was
practiced. This finding disagrees with previous observations by Benson
et al. (12), Ziegler et al. (14),
and Gervino and Veazey (13), but is in agreement to
those of others (15-18).
Although the mechanisms involved
in producing a decrease or no change in VO2
remain unclear and invite further investigation, several reasons might
explain this finding. First, there is much still unknown regarding
the proposed integrated hypothalamic response that is hypothesized to be
the relaxation response (3,4,21-23).
In particular, the suggestion that relaxation training and meditative practices
(including most other biobehavioral interventions such as imagery, biofeedback,
and music) result in a decrease in VO2
at rest and during exercise is simply not correct. This is evident
with the preceding discussion that illustrates the equivocal results in
the literature. Also, the case reports study by Benson, Malhorta, Goldman,
Jacobs, and Hopkins (24) in which VO2
increased during advanced meditation illustrate this point. Second,
there is the unanswered question regarding the subjects' difficulty in
eliciting the relaxation response during exercise. The subjects may
state that they are able to elicit the relaxation response during exercise
versus at rest, but there are no studies to support their claim. For some
reason, this point does not appear to have been problematic in the previously
mentioned studies and yet it might be the primary factor causing the lack
of consensus. This question of "the specificity of the relaxation response"
(i.e., if PMR is to be elicited during exercise, then it should be learned
during exercise) needs further investigation.
Third, other factors such as motor unit
recruitment patterns, anatomical and physiological characteristics, aerobic
differences between trained and untrained subjects, the elimination of
unnecessary aspects of running style, variations in training protocols,
and the likelihood that VO2
during submaximal
running cannot be influenced beyond some physiologic limit need to be researched
(25-28).
Fourth, there is the question of how long a psychophysiologic self-regulation
intervention (such as relaxation training) should be practice before using
it to improve running economy. Self-regulation interventions that facilitate
elicitation of the relaxation response take time to engender a shift in
values and beliefs. Thus it might be incorrectly hypothesized that psychophysiologic
self-regulation per se (belief, for example, in PMR) is the agent of change
rather than the time spent learning PMR. Finally, there is the role
of the placebo response. If the researchers and/or subjects are enthusiastic
proponents of psychophysiologic self-regulation (versus being skeptics),
then the results are likely to support their belief than if they did not
believe in the effectiveness of the intervention (29).
Although it is evident from the preceding
discussion that mind and body are inextricably interconnected and that
PMR training can produce physiologic changes at rest (e.g., decreased VO2
and HR), the same physiologic changes during exercise are not as easily
reproducible. As mentioned earlier, VO2
was unchanged although, in general, some authors might suggest that there
was a certain trend for HR reduction (p = 0.07) during the relaxation period
versus Control I. The mean HR difference did not reach significance, which
is in agreement with earlier studies
(12,13,17).
However, given that the subjects had learned to self-regulate specific
physiologic responses at rest, and given that they were committed to eliciting
the relaxation response during exercise, it is unclear why their thoughts
and feelings did not produced the desired physiologic changes. Part
of the problem, again, may be that the exercise itself produces conflicting
feedback. The end result is that the subjects' thoughts, feelings,
and beliefs are simply not strong enough to elicit the relaxation response.
The point being, it is difficult for subjects to focus their attention
on thoughts and feelings learned at rest while also experiencing the exercising
condition.
Where does this leave the researcher?
It is important to point out that physiologic changes other than a decrease
in VO2 and HR occur with PMR training,
and they are also manifestations of the hypothalamic response termed "relaxation."
A shift, for example, in autonomic balance to parasympathetic dominance
is also characterized by significant decreases in Fb
and Ve. Theoretically, a decrease
in the work of breathing at the same VO2
would result in a reduction in the subjects' perception of effort.
The results of this study demonstrate that Fb
and Ve are not fixed at a constant work
intensity. Elicitation of the relaxation response during minutes
11-20 resulted in a significant decrease in Fb
and Ve of 10% and 4%, respectively.
Following the relaxation period (minutes 21-30), Fb
increased by 7% while Ve remained unchanged.
These findings are consistent with the studies by Cadarette et al. (17)
and Gervino et al. (13). As such, the subjects
were able to exercise at the same VO2 throughout
the three exercise periods while breathing a reduced volume of air by 4%
during the elicitation of the relaxation response. The importance
of this finding is underscored by the 10% reduction in Fb,
which set the stage for increased ventilatory efficiency (30).
An equally important finding is that the
relaxation response during steady-state exercise produced a significant
6% decrease in SBP (from 132 to 124 mm Hg) when compared with Control I
without relaxation. Interestingly, when the subjects stopped the
practice of the relaxation response and continued to exercise during Control
II (again, without relaxation), the steady-state exercise was associated
with a significant 6% increase in SBP. Therefore, the elicitation
of the relaxation response per se caused the subjects' SBP to decrease
during exercise. While the mechanism remains to be defined, relaxation
during exercise produced a psychophysiologic state of self-regulation distinct
from that found during exercise alone (Control I and II). This finding
is in agreement with the results reported by Gervino et al. (13).
The decrease in SBP suggests an improved
efficiency of the central circulatory system as evident by the significant
decrease in RPP, which is an established correlate of cardiac work and
myocardial oxygen demand (31). This finding is
particularly important given that exercise HR did not decrease during the
elicitation of the relaxation response, and that it appears to contradict
the notion that PMR training results in a parasympathetic response.
Clearly, in the present study, the SBP response was the primary parasympathetic
measure and, as a component of the RPP calculation, the primary method
by which the work of the heart was reduced at the fixed work load which
illuminates an important point. That is, perhaps, the criterion measure
for improvement in running economy should be the SBP response during the
elicitation of relaxation while exercising and not the VO2
response. Unfortunately, too few researchers have evaluated SBP with
the same interest as VO2, and too few have
looked to other physiologic measures as indices linked to running performance.
Another physiologic measure that may support
the view that PMR is useful in eliciting the the relaxation response
during exercise is the calculation of RER (i.e., the ratio of CO2
produced to O2 consumed at the lungs).
Under steady-state conditions, RER is a reasonably valid measure of respiratory
quotient (RQ, i.e., the ratio of CO2 produced
to O2 consumed at the cells), and is a
means to estimate energy metabolism during exercise. Hence, the usefulness
of RER is that it can serve as a guide to the nutrient mixture being catabolized
for energy. For example, when fat is used for energy, for a given
ATP production, more O2 is required to
oxidize it to CO2 and water than is required
for carbohydrates (32).
An increase in utilization of free fatty
acids to support energy production during submaximal exercise would theoretically
result in an increase in the rate of VO2(33);
a physiologic response not indicative of enhanced running economy.
In fact, as it turns out, RER does not appear to be an important measure
of running economy. Several studies (3,12)
indicate that RER does not change during elicitation of the relaxation
response, which is in agreement with the present study. Gervino et
al.
(13) appears to be the only study reporting that
elicitation of the relaxation response during exercise mediated a significant
decrease in RER. The authors questioned the biologic significance,
however. In fact, if minimization of VO2
is a crucial performance criterion for economical running pattern, a decrease
in RER would not be expected.
The limitations inherent not only in this
study but others like it include: (a) the reliance on the subjects' self-elicited
relaxation response; (b) the effect of the subjects' psychological state
[i.e., affect and mood] on running economy; (c) the relatively short duration
to learn the PMR self-regulation technique; (d) the subjects' perception
and/or belief in whether psychophysiologic self-regulation can influence
the physiologic response during exercise; and (e) the degree to which the
learned elicitation of the relaxation response at rest can be elicited
equally as effective during exercise.
Conclusions
Given the fact that no change in VO2
occurred during the exercise period in which the relaxation response was
elicited indicates the imperturbability of the subjects' exercise metabolism
and thus unchanged running economy as presently defined (34).
It is very tempting to, therefore, question VO2
as the criterion variable for demonstrating changes in running economy.
Clearly the research efforts that have looked at the elicitation of the
relaxation response during exercise have not resulted in sufficient evidence
to warrant the conclusion that exercise metabolism can be systematically
modified. The results of this study reinforce rather the importance
of psychophysiologic self- regulation during exercise on respiratory and
myocardial variables (particularly Fb,
Ve, and RPP). Regarding the latter variable,
there is ample evidence that a reduction in RPP has a positive and unequivocal
beneficial influence on the work of the heart. This consideration
alone may be of more practical significance than the measured VO2
and, theoretically, may enable the performer to better tolerate the central
demand associated with exercise. Based on this interpretation, not unexpectedly
then, the net result is a better performance.
References
1. Dossey,
B.M. Holistic nursing: a handbook for practice (2nd ed.).
Maryland:Aspen Publishers, 1995.
2. Ikemi,
A., Tornita, S., Kuroda, M., Hayashida, Y., & Ikemi, Y. Self-regulation
method: Psychological, physiological, and clinical considerations. Psychotherapy
and Psychosomatics, 1988;40:184-196.
3. Ikemi,
A., Tornita, S., Kuroda, M., Hayashida, Y., & Ikemi, Y. Self-regulation
method: Psychological, physiological, and clinical considerations. Psychotherapy
and Psychosomatics, 1988;40:184-196.
4. Beary,
J.K., & Benson, H. A simple psychophysiologic technique which elicits
hypometabolic changes of the relaxation response. Psychosomatic Medicine,
1974;36:115-121.
5. Benson,
H., Beary, J.F., & Carol, M.P. The relaxation response. Psychiatry,
1974;37:37-46.
6. Hoffman,
J.W., Arns, P.A., Stainbrook, G.L., Landsberg, L., Young, J., & Gill,
A. Reduced sympathetic nervous system responsivity associated with the
relaxation response. Science, 1982;215:190-192.
7. Anand,
B.K, Chinna, G.S., Singh, B. . Studies on Shri Ramanand Yogi during his
stay in an airtight box.International Journal of Medical Research,
1961;49:82-89.
8. Sugi,
Y., & Akutsu, K. Studies on respiration and energy- metabolism during
sitting in Zazen. Research Journal of Physical Education,
1968;12:190-206.
9. Lim,
Y.A., Boone, T., Flarity, J.R., & Thompson, W.R. Effects of Qigong
on cardiorespiratory changes: A preliminary study. American Journal
of Chinese Medicine, 1993;21:1-6.
10. Morgan,
W.P. Psychogenic factors and exercise metabolism: A review. Medicine
and Science in Sports and Exercise, 1985;17:309-316.
11. Crews,
D.J. Psychological state and running economy. Medicine and Science
in Sports and Exercise, 1992;24:475-482.
12. Benson,
H., Dryer, T., & Hartley, L.H. Decreased VO2
consumption during exercise with elicitation of the relaxation response.
Journal of Human Stress, 1978;4:38-42.
13. Gervino,
E.V., & Veazey, A.E. The physiologic effects of Benson's relaxation
response during submaximal aerobic exercise. Journal of Cardiac Rehabilitation,
1984;6:10-12.
14. Ziegler,
S.G., Klinzing, J., & Williamson, K. The effects of two stress management
training programs on cardiorespiratory efficiency. Journal of Sports
Psychology, 1982;4:280-289.
15. Smith,
A.L., Gill, D.L., Crews, D.J., Hopewell, R., & Morgan, D.W. Attentional
strategy use by experienced distance runners: Physiological and psychological
effects. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 1995;66:142-150.
16. Ashley,
E.A., Rajab, E.P., Timmons, J.A., Smith, A.A., & Nutrie, N. Short term
relaxation training can reduce the physiological cost of running.Paper
presented at the 42nd Annual Meeting of the American College of Sports
Medicine, Minneapolis, MN, 1995.
17. Cadarette,
B.S., Hoffman, J.W., Caudill, M., Kutz, I., Levine, L., Benson, H., &
Goldman, R.F. Effects of the relaxation response on selected cardiorespiratory
responses during physical exercise. Paper presented at the 29th Annual
Meeting of the American College of Sports Medicine, Minneapolis,
MN, 1982.
18. Cortes,
C., Boyd, N.R., & Boone, T. Effects of a relaxation session on submaximal
oxygen uptake and hemodynamic measures. The Journal, 1986;6:10-12.
19. Daniels,
J.T. A physiologist's view of running economy. Medicine and Science
in Sports and Exercise, 1985;17:332-338.
20. Pate,
R.R., & Branch, J.D. Training for endurance sport. Medicine and
Science in Sports and Exercise, 1992;24:(Suppl.), S340-S343.
21. Wallace,
R.K., & Benson, H. The physiology of meditation. Scientific American,
1972;226:84-90.
22. Benson,
H., Klemchuk, H.P., & Graham, J.R. The usefulness of the relaxation
response in the therapy of headache. Headache, 1974;14:49-52.
23. Benson,
H. The relaxation response. New York: Morrow Book Company,
1975.
24. Benson,
H., Malhorta, M.S., Goldman, R.F., Jacobs, G.D., & Hopkins, P.J. Three
case reports of the metabolic and electroencephalographic changes during
advanced buddhist meditation techniques. Behavioral Medicine,
1990;(Summer).
25. Bransford,
D., & Howley, E. Oxygen cost of running in trained and untrained men
and women. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 1977;9:41-44.
26. Daniels,
J., Scardina, N., & Foley, P. VO2max
during five modes of exercise. In N. Bachl, L. Prokop, and R. Suckert (Eds.),
Proceedings
of the World Congress on Sports Medicine (pp. 604-615). Vienna:
Urban and Schwartzenberg, 1984.
27. Morgan,
D., & Craib, M. Physiological aspects of running economy.Medicine
and Science in Sports and Exercise, 1992;24:456-461.
28. Morgan,
D.W., Bransford, D.R., Costill, D.L., Daniels, J.T., Howley, E.T., &
Krahenbuhl, G.S. Variation in the aerobic demand of running among trained
and untrained subjects.Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise,
1995;27:404-409.
29. Benson,
H., & McCallie, D.D., Jr. Angina pectoris and the placebo effect.
New
England Journal of Medicine, 1979;300:1424.
30. Bhambhani,
Y., & Singh, M. The effects of three training intensities on VO2max
and Ve/VO2ratio.
Canadian
Journal of Applied Sport Science, 1985;10:44-50.
31. Kitamura,
K., Jorgensen, C.R., Gobel, F.L., Taylor, H.L., & Wang, Y. Hemodynamic
correlates of myocardial oxygen consumption during upright exercise.
Journal of Applied Physiology, 1972;32:516-522.
32. McArdle,
W.D., Katch, F.I., & Katch, Victor L. Exercise physiology: energy,
nutrition, and human performance. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins,
1996.
33. Ramsbottom,
R., Williams, C., Fleming, N., & Nute, M.L.G. Training induced physiological
and metabolic changes associated with improvements in running performance.
British
Journal of Sports Medicine, 1989;23: 171-176.
34. Morgan,
D.W., Martin, P.E., Baldini, F.D., & Krahenbuhl, G.S. Effects of a
prolonged maximal run on running economy and running mechanics.Medicine
and Science in Sports and Exercise, 1990;22:834-840.
Return
to Contents
Copyright ©1998
American
Society of Exercise Physiologists
All Rights Reserved
|